Dynamic Systems Theory: Always in relationship to:
Rose, L. T., & Fischer, K. W. (in press).
Dynamic systems theory. In R. A. Shweder (Ed.), Chicago companion to the child.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Dynamic Systems Theory
L. Todd Rose & Kurt W. Fischer
Diversity is the hallmark of
human behavior: Regardless of age or experience, people’s performance changes
dramatically depending on context, including the presence of different people.
The same sixth-grade student who can easily solve a difficult math problem in
class often cannot solve the same problem at home on her own, or even in class
the next day. These kinds of fluctuations in performance can be frustrating,
but they are normal. The fact is that variation is a part of all human
behavior. Yet despite its pervasiveness, variability has frequently been
ignored in developmental science. As a consequence, the field now brims with
elaborate descriptions about global changes in behavior, but it struggles to
explain why a child can recite the alphabet for his parents, but not his
teacher. In recent years, a number of researchers have emphasized the
importance of variability, and have sought to explain both stability and
diversity in behavior over time. In order to capture the richness and
complexity of development, many researchers have adopted concepts, methods, and
tools from Dynamic Systems Theory—a flexible framework for analyzing how many
factors act together in natural systems in disciplines as diverse as physics,
biology, and education. Over two decades ago, researchers such as Esther
Thelen, Paul van Geert, and Kurt Fischer (and others) helped pioneer the
application of dynamic systems to development. Their work laid the foundation
for a fresh approach to understanding how people learn, grow, and change.
Formally, dynamic systems theory is an abstract framework, based on concepts
from thermodynamics and nonlinear mathematics. However, whereas some of the
concepts (and much 2 of the terminology) may seem foreign to researchers and
practitioners, the principles of dynamic systems theory are very
straightforward, and deeply relevant to the study of human behavior. The
dynamic systems approach in development starts with two principles: (1)
Multiple characteristics of person and context collaborate to produce all
aspects of behavior; and (2) variability in performance provides important
information for understanding behavior and development. Taken together, these
principles—person-in-context and variability-as-information—represent the
backbone of dynamic systems theory. Building on these themes, researchers have
overturned misconceptions and resolved long-standing arguments about the nature
of development. Person and context together In the game of baseball, the
pitcher’s job is to throw the ball for a strike. However, even the best
pitchers cannot do this every time. Why not? The simple answer is that throwing
a ball accurately—like all human behavior—always depends on more than just
biology and experience. Context matters! In fact, so many contextual factors influence
the accuracy of any given pitch— temperature, crowd noise, or having a runner
on base (to name but a few)—the performance of a pitcher cannot be understood
outside the immediate context. This is true for all behaviors, not just tossing
a baseball. Behavior is not something a person ‘has’: it emerges through the
interactions between person and context, and depends on many biological and
contextual factors. Traditional models assume that people have stable skills,
and discount the importance of personin-context – the dynamics of behavior.
However, ignoring these dynamics can lead to serious misconceptions. The case
of the infant stepping reflex illustrates this principle nicely. Newborns have
many primitive reflexes, including the ‘stepping reflex’: the pattern of leg
movements (steps) an infant makes when held upright. Present at birth, this
reflex disappears after a couple of months, only to reappear around the time of
walking—something that puzzled 3 researchers for decades. Classic explanations
for this phenomenon were based on the neurology of infants: Brain areas matured
and then suppressed the newborn reflex. This was the established view until
Thelen, using dynamic systems theory, offered a different explanation,
considering characteristics of infants’ bodies that the brain explanation
treated as irrelevant. The stepping reflex disappeared not because of
neurological changes but because of changes in leg weight: Babies’ legs showed
an increase in mass (subcutaneous fat as well as other tissue) that made it
impossible for an infant to lift his or her legs from an upright position.
Showing the importance of context, Thelen tested infants who had seemingly lost
the reflex by placing them into a tub of water, where the buoyancy reduced the
effective weight of the legs. Now, when the infants were held upright, their
reflex returned and they stepped just like younger infants! By manipulating
non-obvious variables, Thelen was able to control the emergence and suppression
of a reflex once thought to be under strict neurological control. In this case,
the key principle of person-incontext helped researchers discover the
underlying dynamics of infant motor development. Variability as information
People routinely show this kind of variability in behavior, rarely performing
at a single fixed level consistently. All behavior emerges through interactions
between person and context, and thus performance varies dramatically and
systematically depending on many factors such as arousal level, emotional
state, task demands, and assessment conditions (to name a few). In contrast
with classic models, dynamic systems theory treats variability as information,
and seeks patterns and order in the variation. Because variability is analyzed
instead of ignored, researchers are able to identify factors that have a
systematic effect on behavior and development – to find the order in the
variation. Fischer has shown that one powerful source of variability is
contextual support: With priming of key ideas or actions through the help of an
adult or a well-designed 4 computer program or text, a person can perform at a
higher level, but they cannot sustain the performance without such support.
Madison, 16, demonstrates the influence of contextual support through her
understanding of the relation between addition and multiplication—both of which
involve combining numbers to get larger numbers, with addition combining single
numbers and multiplication groups of numbers. This relation is difficult for
adolescents to articulate, but around 15 or 16 years, they can understand the
concept if they have contextual support. In Madison’s case, she has no problem
getting the relation when her teacher prompts the key ideas, and she can even
provide specific examples (5+5+5+5 = 20 and 5 x 4 = 20). However, when
discussing it with her parents or friends, her performance drops
dramatically—for example, she says that addition and multiplication are the
same but cannot explain the essential difference (single numbers versus groups)
that she understood with her teacher’s support. Madison’s performance speaks
against the notion that people have a single level of ability: What would her
‘true’ level be? In reality, Madison clearly varies between two levels of
performance, what Fischer calls the developmental range: She understands the
relation with support, but she does not understand it without support. The
skill is both present and absent, depending on the context. In other words, it
is dynamic! The importance of support is hardly controversial. However, when studied
through the lens of dynamic systems, contextual support can reveal surprising
facts about learning, and it offers a simple solution to the classic debate
about stages. Using nonlinear models from dynamic systems, Fischer studied
patterns of development in conditions of high- and low-support independently.
When growth was assessed under high-support, it showed clear stage-like
properties. However, in low-support conditions growth was smooth and
continuous. Through the 5 careful use of dynamic systems tools, and by treating
variability as information, Fischer was able to show that stages both do and do
not exist, depending on the dynamics of the activity! Conclusion Human behavior
is flexible and dynamic. Whether tossing a baseball or solving a math problem,
behavior comes from more than just the person, and more than just the context:
It is always about the person-in-context. Many different factors (biological
and contextual) influence performance, and this makes behavior both complex and
variable. Any meaningful account of development must be able to explain
behavior in all its richness and variation. Dynamic systems theory offers
powerful concepts and tools both to capture the stability of behavior over time
and to explain why a young child reads better for his parents than his teacher.
Explaining stability and change together is a key strength of dynamic systems
theory. 6 Suggested Readings: Fischer, K. W. & Rose, L. T. (2001). Webs of
skill: How students learn. Educational Leadership, 59(3), 6-12. Fischer, K. W.
& Bidell, T. R. (2006). Dynamic development of action and thought. In W.
Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Theoretical models of human development.
Handbook of child psychology (6th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 313-399). New York: Wiley.
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the
development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Van Geert, P.
& Steenbeek, H. (2005). Explaining after by before: Basic aspects of a
dynamic systems approach to the study of development. Developmental Review, 25,
408-442.
Comments
Post a Comment