Empathy predates thinking and language
Oochy-coochy-ugg: caring predated thinking
Jonathan
Leake Science Editor Published: 22 February 2015
Early humans’ development of caring
behaviour meant they worked better together and so improved each other’s
survival chances. The change drove brain growth and the evolution of
intelligence, scientists suggest.
EARLY humans
developed kindness, compassion and a sense of beauty long before the emergence
of intelligence, new research claims.
As long ago as
3m years australopithecines, which had brains just a third the size of ours,
were carrying pebbles shaped like babies’ faces. By 1.5m years ago, with brains
still only 60% of their size today, Homo ergaster was caring for the ill, while
Homo heidelbergensis, which lived 450,000 years ago, appear to have nursed
disabled children.
By contrast,
intelligence and language skills, as seen in modern humans, are thought to have
emerged only in the past 500,000 years and possibly as late as 150,000 years
ago. “Human evolution is usually depicted as driven by intelligence, with
empathy and deeper emotions following,” said Penny Spikins, who researches
human origins at York University.
‘However, the
evidence suggests it happened the other way round. Evolution made us sociable,
living in groups and looking after one another, even before we had language.
Our success since then, including the evolution of intelligence, all sprang
from that.’
Spikins, who
is publishing her findings in a book, How Compassion Made Us Human, points to archaeological
finds hinting that even pre-humans had unsuspected emotional depths. Among the
earliest is the Makapansgat pebble, a small rock with pits and markings
resembling a baby’s face, found in South African cave with australopithecines,
dating back 3m years.
Australopithecines,
our direct ancestors, were widely depicted as the ‘killer ape’ because skulls
and bones in the cave had been smashed in what the 1930’s archaeologists who
found them assumed was early warfare. The discovery indpired the early scenes
in the film 2001: A Space Odyssey, which suggested that fighting helped apes to
evolve into humans. But Spikins says australopithecines were hunted by other
animals and survived by co-operating rather than fighting.
‘What is
remarkable is that this pebble was carried several miles back to its cave by an
australopithecine. Did it remind them of a baby? It is impossible to tell for sure
but this is not the only tantalising sign of something perhaps approaching
tenderness,’ she said.
Other signs include
tracks made in east Africa 3.5 million years ago that show two adults were followed
over an ash field by a child walking, apparently playfully, in their
footprints.
By 1.6m years
ago the evidence gets stronger. this includes the remains of a female Homo
erectus in Kenya who was cared for during a long illness, and a site, and a
site of similar age in Dmanisi, Georgia, which yielded the skull of someone who
lived for years without any teeth,
implying that they had help getting food soft enough to eat. By 450,000 years
ago there is more evidence of empathetic behaviour, with groups of Homo
heidelbergensis caring for disabled youngsters, including a child with learning
difficulties whose deformed skull was found in Spain.
Spikins said
early humans may have had a sense of aesthetics. This is suggested by a 250,000
year old handaxe found at West Tofts in Norfolk, made from rock containing a
fossilised scallop shell and designed to make the fossil the centrepiece of the
tool.
‘A uniquely
human feeling lies behind both the creation of such finely crafted tools and caring
for the vulnerable.’ Spikins said.
‘It suggests
early humans, from 2m years ago, were emotionally similar to us.’
Her theories
are likely to be controversial in a field dominated by the belief that early
humans were ruthless huntress, supporting their own family group but often
killing others. Such notions are supported by the discovery of the remains of a
Neanderthal cannibal feast in northern Spain dating back 49,000 years.
Spikins says
while fighting and competition did happen, they were less important than the
co-operation that helped drive the evolution of intelligence. ‘The idea that we
are all innately selfish, which comes from modern economics, has had a strong
influence on how we interpret archaeological finds but the evidence suggests
that …early humans’ survival would have depended on co-operation: aggressive or
selfish behaviour would have been very risky,’ she said.
‘It could be
that our society, dominated by self-interest, is not the pinnacle of evolution
at all but a novel and potentially dangerous social experiment.’
‘
Comments
Post a Comment